Still working
through the linkages of The Undoing Project, Behavioral Finance and Valuation
As I prepare for
the spring semester – and a new module on Behavioral Finance – I must admit that
I’m still somewhat of a skeptic on the topic following my reading of The Undoing Project (See my previous blog entry on the review of Michael Lewis’ The Undoing Project). While each year more research is providing interesting results about the irrational
behavior of individual investors and financial analysts, I remain committed to
the ‘rationality’ assumptions that support classical finance theory and the belief in the efficiency of markets in
the long-run.
I don’t want to
ignore the reality that all individuals do not always act as wealth maximizing,
risk adverse investors - and that we weigh losses more than equivalent gains. So my conclusion is that economists need to better specify the von Neumann-Morgenstern expected utility function; however, that's for another time...
What has me interested in behavioral finance is its ability to hopefully improve investor performance. The annual Dalbar study, which follows the overall performance of the ‘average’ investor, certainly backs up the need for improvement. Note the dismal performance of average investors (yellow bar) over a recent 20-year period (the graphic below shows average annual returns of only 2.1% versus 7.2% for a 60/40 portfolio). [The table is from JP Morgan’s Q1 2017 Guide to the Markets].
What has me interested in behavioral finance is its ability to hopefully improve investor performance. The annual Dalbar study, which follows the overall performance of the ‘average’ investor, certainly backs up the need for improvement. Note the dismal performance of average investors (yellow bar) over a recent 20-year period (the graphic below shows average annual returns of only 2.1% versus 7.2% for a 60/40 portfolio). [The table is from JP Morgan’s Q1 2017 Guide to the Markets].
20-Year Annualized Average Performance Indicates Poor Returns for the 'Average Investor' |
So, to be clear, I
believe it is important for students of finance to understand the common behavioral investment mistakes made by individual investors and analysts (i.e. framing, overconfidence,
anchoring, etc.). If they can learn to avoid these mistakes – and even better yet, help
their clients avoid them as well - then it is a worthwhile endeavor. It would
be great if over time the ‘average investor’ earned returns more in line with
the 60/40 returns shown above – it would help minimize some of the concern
about the looming retirement crisis forecast for Western economies.
I realize that the
real world is untidy and not every investor or financial analyst has the same information
or acumen to act rationally, so learning some basic rules of thumb (or heuristics) of behavioral finance might help some individuals avoid serious blunders. That’s a
good thing.
I do find the chore
of reconciling behavioral finance with traditional discounted cash (DCF) valuation
to be challenging. Each semester I teach my students how to conduct fundamental
investment analysis using various valuation approaches (DCF models, arbitrage
pricing, and relative valuation). I continue to believe that the models hold up
well and that there is a substantial body of empirical research that supports long-term market
efficiency; so, I am not about to abandon classical finance theory.
As behavioral
finance continues to make major inroads in academia and the investment industry, I don't believe that traditional intrinsic
valuation approaches will change much at all. True, the expected future cash flows
and the required discount rate in a DCF model still must be forecast; however, if an
analyst has skewed or biased expectations that lead them to inaccurate
valuations, then they’ll eventually be working in at another occupation.
It can be argued
that assessing future cash flows is an undertaking that is subject to all sorts
of behavioral deceptions and traps; however, in the end the truth comes out and
either the analyst has a good batting average or not. So, while
traditional, fundamental analysis can be rife with behavioral issues, the valuation
process is worth doing. The fine line is to avoid being too mechanical versus
too instinctive. Forming expectations
and adjusting for risk can be skewed by behavioral components; however, the key
to doing good valuation work is to be aware of your own behavioral biases, how
they might impact the modeling, and to learn how to counteract your prejudices.
Here is where I
believe behavioral finance has value for investment students and future research analysts:
- It can help explain why analysts arrive at different prices for the same stock. By studying the interaction between psychology and valuation, behavioral finance can help to identify systematic analysis errors (i.e. herding, availability bias, hindsight bias, etc.) in the valuation process.
- It is useful in understanding why a stock price differs from an analyst’s estimate of intrinsic value. The price can diverge from the intrinsic value because the analyst makes estimation mistakes or because they missed or under-weighted some relevant information. The analyst could behave in an irrational manner (refusing to deviate from the crowd or exhibiting overconfidence, familiarity or home bias) that can cause prices to depart significantly from their estimated values.
- Overconfidence can be detrimental to the analyst’s stock-picking ability in the long run and can lead to excessive trading. Analysts, like any other professional, tend to exaggerate their abilities and underestimate the likelihood of bad outcomes over which they have no control. This combination of overconfidence and optimism can cause analysts to overestimate the reliability of their knowledge, underestimate risks and exaggerate their ability to control events, which often leads to excessive trading.
The one area of behavioral finance
that I concede is not properly or adequately addressed by classical “rationality’
economics is loss aversion. This refers to an individual's tendencies to prefer
avoiding losses over acquiring equivalent gains. In common terms it could be
said that most people feel it is better to not lose $100, than it would be to
gain $100. Amos Tversky and Daniel Kahneman conducted studies and suggested that losses are at least twice as powerful, psychologically, as are equal gains. This observation (loss aversion) needs to be incorporated into expected utility theory.
As an investment professor, it behooves me to help my students become conversant with the concepts and findings of behavioralists so that they can recognize when their instincts or prejudices could lead to poor outcomes. The CFA Institute is also adding more content on behavioral finance.
As an investment professor, it behooves me to help my students become conversant with the concepts and findings of behavioralists so that they can recognize when their instincts or prejudices could lead to poor outcomes. The CFA Institute is also adding more content on behavioral finance.
So, you ask, if most
analysts and investors have behavioral biases, then how can markets be
efficient? The answer for me is clear... while any analyst can be wrong about their price estimate for Amazon, the many
independent analysts who cover Amazon are likely to collectively get the valuation correct, on average, in
the long-term – the trick as an analyst or investor is to have fewer missed estimates!